All seminars are Tuesday at 12:05 pm in 4274
Chamberlin Hall except as noted. Refreshments will be served.
Short List
Jan 22, 2013 - George Hrabovsky, Madison Area Science and
Technology
Jan 29, 2013 - Paula Niedenthal, Psychology
Feb 5, 2013 - Patricia McConnell, Zoology
Feb 12, 2013 - Charles Franklin, Political Science
Feb 19, 2013 - Joel Robbin, Mathematics
Feb 26, 2013 - Dominique Brossard, Life Sciences and
Communication
Mar 5, 2013 - John Hawks, Anthropology
Mar 12, 2013 - Brian Harvey, Zoology
Mar 19, 2013 - Art Schmaltz, Prairie State College
Mar 26, 2013 - NO SEMINAR (spring break)
Apr 2, 2013 - Cher Li, Economics
Apr 9, 2013 - Tim Rogers, Psychology
Apr 16, 2013 - Tony Stretton, Zoology
Apr 23, 2013 - Lewis Leavitt, Pediatrics
Apr 30, 2013 - Stan Temple, Nelson Institute
May 7, 2013 - Clint Sprott, Physics
Join us for lunch during the summer on the Memorial
Union Terrace at noon each Tuesday, starting May 14th!
Abstracts
January 22, 2013
The Minus-1st Law: The conservation of information and how it leads
to chaos
George Hrabovsky, Madison Area Science and Technology
The conservation of information is central to all of physics. It
also seems so obvious to us physicists that we don't talk about it
much in courses. It is a simple idea, the quantity of information in
a closed system never changes. This simple idea requires us to think
deeply about the nature of dynamical systems. The ramifications are
dramatic; it leads to the second law of thermodynamics and to chaos.
January 29, 2013
What do smiles mean and how do we know?
Paula M. Niedenthal, UW Department of Psychology
Theories of embodied emotion suggest new ways to model the
recognition of facial expression. Behavioral and neuroimaging
studies indicate that the recognition of facial expressions of
emotion, and in particular the elusive smile, involves the
(re)production of the expression as well as of the corresponding
emotion, or parts of it, in the self. In the present talk, I
introduce a new model, The Simulation of Smiles Model (SIMS,
Niedenthal et al., BBS, 2010). The SIMS relies on a
social-functional typology of smiles. Accordingly, I first present
research that seeks to validate the typology. The SIMS also outlines
specific roles for facial mimicry and eye contact in representing
smile meaning. Recent empirical evidence in favor of these roles is
presented. Finally, the SIMS leaves room for the use of perceptual
and conceptual processes in interpreting facial expression. I
present research supportive of the hypothesis that the
interpretation of smile meaning relies on prior beliefs and
stereotypes when facial mimicry does not occur. Results of a recent
study on smiling behavior from 9 different countries provides the
basis for predictions about moderation by culture of the basic
processes outlined in SIMS.
February 5, 2013
Are you thinking what I'm thinking?
Patricia B McConnell, UW Department of Zoology
It's hard enough to know what's going on inside the head of your
best friend, much less a member of another species. But recent
research by psychologists and neurobiologists have a lot to tell us
about the mental lives of non-human animals. If you've ever wondered
what's going on inside heads of the animals you live with, join
Patricia McConnell in an inquiry into the emotional and cognition
lives of mammals. The talk will include a look at how the emotional
lives of mammals & birds might compare with ours, their
comparative problem solving ability and the controversial concept of
"Theory of Mind."
February 12, 2013
Was 2012 a failure for the polls? Was Nate Silver exceptionally
accurate?
Charles Franklin, UW Department of Political Science
In 2012 polls came in for exceptional scrutiny and criticism.
Claims that polls where hopelessly skewed became a common meme of
the fall campaign. And indeed, polling faces significant practical
challenges. But did the polls, in the end, perform poorly?
Interestingly, campaign leaders from both the Obama and the Romney
campaign have been quoted saying the polls failed. And yet the Obama
campaign made unprecedented use of polling in their data analytics.
Meanwhile, Nate Silver came to personify the quantitative analysis
of election campaigns. His successful predictions for 50 of 50
states is vindication of his forecasts. But did Nate do better than
other forecasts?
February 19, 2013
Thom's catastrophe theory and Zeeman's model of the stock market
Joel Robbin, UW Department of Mathematics
Catastrophe theory is a method for describing the evolution of forms
in nature. It was discovered by Ren´e Thom in the 1960’s. Thom
expounded the philosophy behind the theory in his 1972 book
Structural stability and morphogenesis. Catastrophe theory is
particularly applicable where gradually changing forces produce
sudden effects. The applications of catastrophe theory in classical
physics (or more generally in any subject governed by a
‘minimization principle’) are noncontroversial and help us
understand what diverse models have in common. The applications of
the theory in the social and biological sciences have met with some
resistance. (I don’t know if any workers in these areas have been
influenced by Thom’s ideas.) In this talk I will discuss three
examples: Zeeman’s toy (the “catastrophe machine”), light caustics,
and Zeeman’s explanation of stock market booms and busts.
The constantly evolving slides for this talk are available on my website.
February 26, 2013
The brave cyberworld of science communication
Dominique Brossard, UW Department of Life Sciences and Communication
As more and more individuals turn to online environments to follow
scientific issues and find information about science, recent
research is science communication has focused on examining how these
online environments may shape public attitudes toward science and
public understanding of science. This introduction will discuss
patterns of science communication online and present recent research
findings in that area. Notably, I will introduce results of an
experiment with a national sample of the American population testing
the effects of comments on science blogs on readers’ attitudes
toward the scientific issues covered in such blogs. I will also
explain the effects of current practices related to online searches
on public understanding of science. In light of these results,
challenges and opportunities for science as an institution as well
as for science and society will be discussed.
March 5, 2013
How are complex adaptations built? Using cultural and genetic
convergence to understand evolving systems
John Hawks, UW Department of Anthropology
Adaptation by natural selection is a genetically heterogeneous
process. Some adaptive phenotypes are the result of simple genetic
changes under positive natural selection. But some adaptive
phenotypes are more complex, requiring changes to a network of
interacting genes, possibly in addition to gene-environment
interactions. Is there any general process by which such complex
adaptations can be understood, or are they a simple stochastic
accumulation of simple changes? The record of recent human evolution
provides a wealth of cases of genetic and cultural changes that have
unfolded convergently in different populations. Genetic adaptation
to new pathogens, new diets and new physical environments allows us
to probe the networks of genetic interactions and the timing of
changes on multiple human genes. Cultural adaptation to new diets
and modes of social organization also allow us to examine how
evolutionary dynamics may constrain the path taken by complex
adaptations. I lay out a research agenda that distinguishes
functional networks from evolutionary networks, giving us a way to
discuss the origins of complexity through evolutionary time.
March 12, 2013
Climate change, disturbance, and forest resilience
Brian Harvey, UW Department of Zoology
The direct and indirect consequences of climate change on forests of
North America are only beginning to unfold. Tree health will be
directly affected by changing temperature and precipitation
patterns. However, just as important are the effects of a changing
climate on many of the natural disturbance processes such as
wildfire and insect outbreaks that have shaped forest ecosystems for
millennia. As trees are relatively long-lived organisms,
climate-driven changes to forest ecosystems may be subtle until a
disturbance catalyzes change and sends the system along a new
trajectory. This talk will include a look into what we can expect in
western forests under new climatic and disturbance regimes.
March 19, 2013
The textual structure of REM dreaming
Art Schmaltz, Prairie State College
The human brain during REM dreaming is a singular neurological event
that may well be the most complex event known to science. As a
biologically evolved system, dreaming long predates the evolution of
human language.
In this presentation, I will parse out one of the ten lines of
evidence that argues that human language evolved "down" from the
complexity of dreaming, and not "up" from a simpler biological
system.
April 2, 2013
The effects of human capital depreciation on occupational gender
segregation
Hsueh-Hsiang (Cher) Li, UW Department of Economics
This paper analyzes how human capital depreciation affects
occupational gender segregation. Prior studies are generally biased
because, given an occupational depreciation rate, female workers
endogenously choose the duration of leave. I address this problem by
proposing an alternative depreciation measure utilizing involuntary
job displacement shocks. Using this depreciation proxy along with
additional pecuniary and non-pecuniary occupational attributes, I
estimate a conditional logit model of occupational choices
separately for male and female college graduates. My results show
that women have a stronger distaste than men for occupations with
high human capital depreciation.
April 9, 2013
Why do people believe crazy things?
Tim Rogers, UW Department of Psychology
Theories of human knowledge acquisition (ie learning) vary in
many of their particulars but typically embrace the common
assumption that learning is rational: through learning, people
acquire reasonably accurate statistical models of the environment
that allow them, given some new information, to make approximately
optimal probabilistic inferences about unobserved states of the
world. My own work on knowledge acquisition resides firmly in this
tradition, but I have always found it difficult to reconcile this
view with the everyday observation that many people appear to
pretty firmly believe some pretty crazy things. We can see
that this is true even without having to agree what the crazy
beliefs are. For instance, the President either was or was not
born in Hawaii. These are the only two logical possibilities, and
there is a fact of the matter. Of the two groups prepared to
vociferously argue each side of the proposition, one must be
wrong. The incorrect belief persists in this group despite the
fact that we all live to some extent in the same world and are
presumably applying largely similar reasoning mechanisms to bear
on largely the same evidence. The same point can be made with
reference to controversies about global warming, evolution,
whether vaccines cause autism, the efficacy of trickle-down
economics or gun control policy, the relative payscales of public
and private sector workers, and any number of other important
issues facing public life. If we are all such optimal learners,
why do people arrive at such starkly opposing sets of beliefs?
There is a long tradition of research addressing aspects of this
problem. One idea is that human reasoning is "motivated"--there
are emotional costs associated with different beliefs, and in
deciding which beliefs to endorse, people jointly minimize an
error cost (ie, fit of the beliefs to evidence) and the emotional
cost associated with the belief. But this approach fails to
address the central question of where the emotional cost comes
from, or why people should be "motivated" to entertain incorrect
beliefs in the first place. A second hypothesis is that the
cognitive mechanisms that support human learning and inference
were only optimal in an evolutionary context, and are not suited
to the modern environment in which we now find ourselves. But such
accounts seem similarly underconstrained without some specific
accounting of what the learning mechanisms are and how and why
specifically they are unsuited to our current environment.
I'd like to discuss a third possibility that arises from a
consideration of the dynamics of learning in social groups. Models
of human learning typically view the learner as an independent
entity living in an environment that provides independent samples
from a static distribution, occasionally or even frequently paired
with category labels provided by an "oracle" that is always
correct. In the real world, the labels we receive are not provided
by an all-knowing oracle, but by other individuals who may
themselves have mistaken or uncertain beliefs, or may be trying to
deceive us. When learners are not viewed as independent and
passive statistical engines, but as agents who communicate their
own current beliefs, the problem of what constitutes the "correct"
rational behavior changes. I will illustrate two simple examples
of cases where individual learning models are "coupled" by having
each learner occasionally provide labels to, and occasionally
receive labels from, other learners in a social group, in addition
to occasionally directly observing the correct label. The beliefs
formed across the group thus constitute a dynamical system, which
can exhibit different steady-states depending upon how each
learner decides to weight information coming from other sources.
In some circumstances the group all converges to the truth; in
other cases, it fractionates into communities that endorse very
different beliefs. To the best of my understanding, I think this
formulation differs from cooperative, competitive, and
communication games, but I expect it bears resemblance, and may
even be identical to, other kinds of dynamical systems, and I am
interested in getting feedback about analogous problems in other
domains. I will also describe new empirical evidence concerning
how human beings weight evidence coming from multiple different
conflicting sources, depending upon their own current beliefs.
This evidence suggests that people may indeed fall prey to the
problematic dynamics exhibited by the simplified learning models.
April 16, 2013
The Ascaris nervous system - a simple nervous system. Hah!
Tony Stretton, UW Department of Zoology
Numerically, nematodes have very simple nervous systems. The
female parasitic nematode Ascaris suum has only 298 neurons, and the
hermaphroditic free-living Caenorhabditis elegans has 302. A.
suum is large (ca 35 cm), and has large neurons suitable for
electrophysiological recording. We assembled a
functional circuit from the morphological synapses, scored by
electron microscopy, and the physiological properties of the neurons
and their synapses. The predicted activity of this circuit
matched that actually recorded from neurons in dissected
preparations that were opened to allow microelectrode
penetration. However, it differed dramatically from the
activity recorded from these same neurons in semi-intact behaving
preparations. Something was missing from the circuit
description. We have now shown that there are numerous
neuropeptides (at least 250) present in A. suum, and the ones we
have sequenced have potent activity on individual neurons.
We think that they were washed out of the dissected
preparations, thus losing their modulatory activity on individual
neurons. For peptide identification, initially peptides were
purified by HPLC and sequenced by Edman degradation. Now we
are using mass spectrometry, which has speeded up the discovery
process more than one hundred-fold. In particular, we
are now dissecting single identified neurons and subjecting them to
MALDI-TOF MS and tandem MS for sequence determination. All
neurons examined so far contain peptides. Most contain
previously unknown peptides, and the unknown peptides often
outnumber the known peptides. This is a powerful method
of peptide discovery. It has the distinct advantage that
it simultaneously solves the identity and the cellular expression of
the peptide. It also has the advantage that it identifies the
peptide actually expressed by a particular neuron, rather than
relying on predictions from cDNA or genomic DNA sequences, and on
reporter constructs for expression patterns. Neuropeptides are
processed from precursor proteins, and the rules of the proteolytic
cleavage are not yet robust enough for accurate prediction of
processing.
April 23, 2013
Reading Mayan Glyphs : a story of politics ,adventure, cultural
misunderstanding and scholarship Lewis Leavitt, UW Department of Pediatrics
In 1843 an American diplomat John Lloyd Stephens published an
account of his travels in Central America which described
extraordinary cities and monuments which were the remnants of an
ancient Mayan civilization. On these monuments were a form of
writing which was beautiful to behold but undecipherable. The story
of the decipherment of this writing by non-Mayans has a history
which dates from the Spanish conquest of Mesoamerica. A Spanish
bishop Diego de Landa Calderon had made great strides in attempting
decipherment in 1566 but due to a tragic cultural misunderstanding
failed to see the key which had been revealed. In the 20th
century Eric Thompson made great strides in deciphering Mayan
calendric dating but when Yuri Knosorov made the crucial
determination of the phonetic basis for Mayan writing in 1952
Soviet-American mistrust, isolation of Soviet scholars, and
“received wisdom” of American experts led to his insights being
ignored for decades. It was not until the 1970s and 80s that the
phonetic decipherment of Mayan writing led to a new understanding of
Mayan civilization and the texts left by that civilization.
April 30, 2013
Aldo Leopold, phenology and climate change
Stan Temple, Nelson Institute
Aldo Leopold, best known
as the author of A Sand County Almanac, was a keen
observer of the natural world. Throughout his life he kept daily
journals recording observations of seasonal events,
especially those occurring at his beloved "shack" on the Leopold
farm which was the setting for many essays in A Sand
County Almanac. Leopold's meticulous phenological
observations have provided us with an unparalleled record of when
plants bloomed, birds migrated and other natural events. Analyzing
his historical observations of hundreds of natural events as well
as recent records helps us understand how climate change
is affecting the ecological community.
May 7, 2013
Lessons learned from 19 years of chaos and complexity
Clint Sprott, UW Department of Physics
As we conclude the nineteenth year of the Chaos and Complex Systems
Seminar, I would like to discuss some of the lessons I have learned
from listening to over 500 talks, from my own research, and from the
many books and articles I have read on the subject. This will be a
rather personal and subjective talk and thus probably controversial.
In particular, I will argue that the feedback, nonlinearities, and
self-organization that characterize all real dynamical systems are
more likely to ameliorate the dire consequences that others have
predicted than to exacerbate them as so many fear. This is not a
prediction that our problems will vanish or an argument for ignoring
them. On the contrary, our choices and actions are the means by
which society will reorganize to become even better in the decades
to follow, albeit surely not a Utopia.